You’ve probably heard about the ‘marshmallow test’ for
impulse control. Briefly, young children are given one marshmallow and told
that if they can delay eating it for a few minutes, they’ll get a second marshmallow. The original experiment was conducted in the 60’s at Stanford University, but cognitive scientists and parents have been repeating the experiment ever since. Here's an example you might enjoy.
Why such interest in whether kids can resist a marshmallow? When those children were revisited later in life, the ones who had been able to delay gratification at age four seemed to have some huge advantages. For example, they had higher SAT scores and suffered from less substance abuse. Apparently, being able to postpone eating a tasty treat as a preschooler has implications for one’s success in life. Or does it? Not so fast.
Celeste Kidd, Holly Palmeri and Richard Aslin of the
University of Rochester repeated the marshmallow test, but with a twist. This
time, prior to giving children their first marshmallow, they set up scenarios
where a facilitator was seen as either ‘reliable’ or ‘unreliable’. Children
were offered a handful of crayons or stickers for a project and told that if
they could wait, the staffer would go and fetch a much better collection of
materials. For one group of kids, the adult did indeed return with some particularly enticing supplies
(reliable). For the second group of
children, the adult came back empty handed (unreliable), explaining that there
weren’t any more materials after all. After two experiences with the adult
being either reliable or unreliable, the kids were presented with their
marshmallows and the real test began.
The kids who had been paired with a reliable facilitator
were able to wait an average of four times longer than the children who had
been twice disappointed by an unreliable adult. This difference may have been
underestimated by the fact that each test was terminated after 15 minutes, by
which point only one out of fourteen children in the unreliable group but nine
out fourteen in the reliable group still had their marshmallow. In other words,
many of the kids in the reliable group might have been willing to wait far
longer.
While young children clearly have difficulty with impulse
control, this study contradicts the view that differences in that control are
purely innate. Instead, the data suggest that the majority of young children
use a rational process to decide whether or not to wait for a reward. After
all, if you can’t trust someone to bring you a second marshmallow, you might as
well go ahead and eat the one in front of you.
The University of Rochester researchers explain:
No comments:
Post a Comment